Congratulations!

[Valid RSS] This is a valid RSS feed.

Recommendations

This feed is valid, but interoperability with the widest range of feed readers could be improved by implementing the following recommendations.

Source: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/rss/journal/10.1111/%28ISSN%291475-6811

  1. <?xml version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"?>
  2. <rss xmlns:atom="http://www.w3.org/2005/Atom"
  3.     xmlns:content="http://purl.org/rss/1.0/modules/content/"
  4.     xmlns:dc="http://purl.org/dc/elements/1.1/"
  5.     xmlns:prism="http://prismstandard.org/namespaces/basic/2.0/"
  6.     version="2.0">
  7.   <channel>
  8.      <title>Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</title>
  9.      <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R</link>
  10.      <description>Table of Contents for Personal Relationships. List of articles from both the latest and EarlyView issues.</description>
  11.      <language>en-US</language>
  12.      <copyright>© International Association for Relationship Research</copyright>
  13.      <managingEditor>wileyonlinelibrary@wiley.com (Wiley Online Library)</managingEditor>
  14.      <pubDate>Fri, 03 May 2024 07:22:54 +0000</pubDate>
  15.      <lastBuildDate>Fri, 03 May 2024 07:22:54 +0000</lastBuildDate>
  16.      <generator>Atypon® Literatum™</generator>
  17.      <docs>https://validator.w3.org/feed/docs/rss2.html</docs>
  18.      <ttl>10080</ttl>
  19.      <dc:title>Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</dc:title>
  20.      <dc:publisher>Wiley</dc:publisher>
  21.      <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  22.      <atom:link href="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R"
  23.                 rel="self"
  24.                 type="application/atom+xml"/>
  25.      <image>
  26.         <title>Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</title>
  27.         <url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/pb-assets/journal-banners/14756811.jpg</url>
  28.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R</link>
  29.      </image>
  30.      <item>
  31.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12547?af=R</link>
  32.         <pubDate>Sun, 28 Apr 2024 23:32:56 -0700</pubDate>
  33.         <dc:date>2024-04-28T11:32:56-07:00</dc:date>
  34.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  35.         <prism:coverDate/>
  36.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  37.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12547</guid>
  38.         <title>Ignorance is not always bliss: A qualitative study of young Adults' experiences with being ghosted</title>
  39.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  40.         <dc:description>
  41. Abstract
  42. Ghosting, or ignoring someone to end a relationship, is often considered a harmful aspect of growing digital connectivity. However, research on ghosting has focused on the experiences of European/European Americans in romantic contexts. Thus, we broadly explored experiences of being ghosted among 29 undergraduates primarily from underrepresented populations (75.9% Latinx, 6.9% Asian, 6.9% Black) in the United States. We conducted face‐to‐face semi‐structured interviews regarding general experiences with being ghosted, a memorable experience of being ghosted, and attitudes toward ghosting. Inductive thematic analysis yielded eight themes surrounding the process of being ghosted (Sensing Shifts in Communication Patterns), ghosting attributions (Unreciprocated Feelings, Incompatibility), responses to ghosting (Stages of Grief, Various Coping Methods, Lost Respect for Ghoster, Insight from Experiencing Both Sides), and ghosting attitudes (Good Reasons). Future research might examine the role of both individual (e.g., cultural values) and situational (e.g., ghosting circumstance) factors in various trajectories of ghosting responses, as well as the psychological consequences of these trajectories.
  43. </dc:description>
  44.         <content:encoded>
  45. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  46. &lt;p&gt;Ghosting, or ignoring someone to end a relationship, is often considered a harmful aspect of growing digital connectivity. However, research on ghosting has focused on the experiences of European/European Americans in romantic contexts. Thus, we broadly explored experiences of being ghosted among 29 undergraduates primarily from underrepresented populations (75.9% Latinx, 6.9% Asian, 6.9% Black) in the United States. We conducted face-to-face semi-structured interviews regarding general experiences with being ghosted, a memorable experience of being ghosted, and attitudes toward ghosting. Inductive thematic analysis yielded eight themes surrounding the process of being ghosted (Sensing Shifts in Communication Patterns), ghosting attributions (Unreciprocated Feelings, Incompatibility), responses to ghosting (Stages of Grief, Various Coping Methods, Lost Respect for Ghoster, Insight from Experiencing Both Sides), and ghosting attitudes (Good Reasons). Future research might examine the role of both individual (e.g., cultural values) and situational (e.g., ghosting circumstance) factors in various trajectories of ghosting responses, as well as the psychological consequences of these trajectories.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  47.         <dc:creator>
  48. Karen Wu,
  49. Olajide Bamishigbin
  50. </dc:creator>
  51.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  52.         <dc:title>Ignorance is not always bliss: A qualitative study of young Adults' experiences with being ghosted</dc:title>
  53.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12547</dc:identifier>
  54.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  55.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12547</prism:doi>
  56.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12547?af=R</prism:url>
  57.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  58.      </item>
  59.      <item>
  60.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12544?af=R</link>
  61.         <pubDate>Mon, 15 Apr 2024 18:18:41 -0700</pubDate>
  62.         <dc:date>2024-04-15T06:18:41-07:00</dc:date>
  63.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  64.         <prism:coverDate/>
  65.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  66.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12544</guid>
  67.         <title>Longitudinal dyadic interplay between marital conflict and psychological well‐being in couples: The moderating roles of Wives' employment</title>
  68.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  69.         <dc:description>
  70. Abstract
  71. This study examined longitudinal dyadic and within‐partner associations among self‐esteem, subjective happiness, and marital conflict in married couples, with a possible moderating role of wives' employment status. Data were analyzed from the Panel Study on Korean Children, nationwide longitudinal data. The study sample included 1668 married couples (N = 3336 participants) where both partners provided separate data annually across three waves. Husbands' and wives' mean ages at T1 were 39.3 and 36.8 years, respectively. For both partners, self‐esteem and subjective happiness were related bidirectionally at an individual level. Wives' marital conflict was linked to husbands' subsequent marital conflict and vice versa. Wives played a greater role in their husbands’ self‐esteem than vice versa. Findings suggest that wives tend to be their husband's substantial source of perceived psychological well‐being than the reverse; however, the benefit of psychological well‐being is likely to be lower for husbands of stay‐at‐home wives.
  72. </dc:description>
  73.         <content:encoded>
  74. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  75. &lt;p&gt;This study examined longitudinal dyadic and within-partner associations among self-esteem, subjective happiness, and marital conflict in married couples, with a possible moderating role of wives' employment status. Data were analyzed from the Panel Study on Korean Children, nationwide longitudinal data. The study sample included 1668 married couples (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 3336 participants) where both partners provided separate data annually across three waves. Husbands' and wives' mean ages at T1 were 39.3 and 36.8 years, respectively. For both partners, self-esteem and subjective happiness were related bidirectionally at an individual level. Wives' marital conflict was linked to husbands' subsequent marital conflict and vice versa. Wives played a greater role in their husbands’ self-esteem than vice versa. Findings suggest that wives tend to be their husband's substantial source of perceived psychological well-being than the reverse; however, the benefit of psychological well-being is likely to be lower for husbands of stay-at-home wives.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  76.         <dc:creator>
  77. Jeong Jin Yu
  78. </dc:creator>
  79.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  80.         <dc:title>Longitudinal dyadic interplay between marital conflict and psychological well‐being in couples: The moderating roles of Wives' employment</dc:title>
  81.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12544</dc:identifier>
  82.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  83.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12544</prism:doi>
  84.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12544?af=R</prism:url>
  85.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  86.      </item>
  87.      <item>
  88.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12545?af=R</link>
  89.         <pubDate>Thu, 11 Apr 2024 23:28:56 -0700</pubDate>
  90.         <dc:date>2024-04-11T11:28:56-07:00</dc:date>
  91.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  92.         <prism:coverDate/>
  93.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  94.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12545</guid>
  95.         <title>Simple reflection exercises can build efficacy and reduce distress about relationship conflicts</title>
  96.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  97.         <dc:description>
  98. Abstract
  99. Serious conflicts in close personal relationships can be highly distressing and tempting to ignore, but avoidance of conflict is maladaptive. In the present research, we tested the effectiveness of short conflict‐reflection interventions to promote constructive engagement with conflicts. In Study 1 (N = 358), a relatively unstructured, conflicted‐reflection intervention significantly reduced distress and bolstered confidence in partners' ability to resolve their relationship conflicts. Study 2 (N = 411) further revealed that this intervention was as, or nearly as effective as more elaborate interventions that prescribed specific, theory‐based, therapeutic elements. Together, results reveal that even brief episodes of constructive reflection on relationship conflicts can improve confidence and reduce distress about them. The positive effects of a relatively unstructured reflection suggest people already have adaptive intuitions about how to more effectively manage conflicts in their relationships and can benefit from brief reflections on how to apply them.
  100. </dc:description>
  101.         <content:encoded>
  102. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  103. &lt;p&gt;Serious conflicts in close personal relationships can be highly distressing and tempting to ignore, but avoidance of conflict is maladaptive. In the present research, we tested the effectiveness of short conflict-reflection interventions to promote constructive engagement with conflicts. In Study 1 (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 358), a relatively unstructured, conflicted-reflection intervention significantly reduced distress and bolstered confidence in partners' ability to resolve their relationship conflicts. Study 2 (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 411) further revealed that this intervention was as, or nearly as effective as more elaborate interventions that prescribed specific, theory-based, therapeutic elements. Together, results reveal that even brief episodes of constructive reflection on relationship conflicts can improve confidence and reduce distress about them. The positive effects of a relatively unstructured reflection suggest people already have adaptive intuitions about how to more effectively manage conflicts in their relationships and can benefit from brief reflections on how to apply them.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  104.         <dc:creator>
  105. Emily M. Britton,
  106. Denise C. Marigold,
  107. Ian McGregor
  108. </dc:creator>
  109.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  110.         <dc:title>Simple reflection exercises can build efficacy and reduce distress about relationship conflicts</dc:title>
  111.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12545</dc:identifier>
  112.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  113.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12545</prism:doi>
  114.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12545?af=R</prism:url>
  115.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  116.      </item>
  117.      <item>
  118.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12541?af=R</link>
  119.         <pubDate>Thu, 04 Apr 2024 16:03:48 -0700</pubDate>
  120.         <dc:date>2024-04-04T04:03:48-07:00</dc:date>
  121.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  122.         <prism:coverDate/>
  123.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  124.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12541</guid>
  125.         <title>Emerging adult siblings' relational entitlement and conflict: The moderating effects of financial dependence on parents</title>
  126.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  127.         <dc:description>
  128. Abstract
  129. Past literature has documented a linkage between entitlement and interpersonal tension, primarily in romantic relationships. However, there is a lack of research investigating the impact of entitlement on sibling relationships. Guided by the agency model of narcissism and adult development literature, this study conceptualized relational entitlement as one form of state narcissism and examined the actor and partner effects of relational entitlement on perceived sibling conflict as well as the moderating effects of financial dependence on parents. Participants were 136 emerging adult sibling dyads (older sibling Mage = 22.20, SD = 2.35; younger sibling Mage = 19.54, SD = 1.55). Results indicated that younger siblings' (not older siblings') relational entitlement was positively associated with both their own and their siblings' perceptions of conflict. Moreover, younger siblings' financial dependence buffered the actor association between younger siblings' relational entitlement and perceived conflict. These findings suggest that emerging adult siblings' developmental status of financial dependence provides a nuanced context for understanding the actor and partner effects of relational entitlement on sibling conflict. The discussion focuses on the role of self‐inflated social comparison in emerging adult sibling conflict and how the developmental characteristics of older and younger siblings shape the above associations.
  130. </dc:description>
  131.         <content:encoded>
  132. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  133. &lt;p&gt;Past literature has documented a linkage between entitlement and interpersonal tension, primarily in romantic relationships. However, there is a lack of research investigating the impact of entitlement on sibling relationships. Guided by the agency model of narcissism and adult development literature, this study conceptualized relational entitlement as one form of state narcissism and examined the actor and partner effects of relational entitlement on perceived sibling conflict as well as the moderating effects of financial dependence on parents. Participants were 136 emerging adult sibling dyads (older sibling &lt;i&gt;M&lt;/i&gt;
  134. &lt;sub&gt;age&lt;/sub&gt; = 22.20, SD = 2.35; younger sibling &lt;i&gt;M&lt;/i&gt;
  135. &lt;sub&gt;age&lt;/sub&gt; = 19.54, SD = 1.55). Results indicated that younger siblings' (not older siblings') relational entitlement was positively associated with both their own and their siblings' perceptions of conflict. Moreover, younger siblings' financial dependence buffered the actor association between younger siblings' relational entitlement and perceived conflict. These findings suggest that emerging adult siblings' developmental status of financial dependence provides a nuanced context for understanding the actor and partner effects of relational entitlement on sibling conflict. The discussion focuses on the role of self-inflated social comparison in emerging adult sibling conflict and how the developmental characteristics of older and younger siblings shape the above associations.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  136.         <dc:creator>
  137. Weimiao Zhou,
  138. Alesia Woszidlo
  139. </dc:creator>
  140.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  141.         <dc:title>Emerging adult siblings' relational entitlement and conflict: The moderating effects of financial dependence on parents</dc:title>
  142.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12541</dc:identifier>
  143.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  144.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12541</prism:doi>
  145.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12541?af=R</prism:url>
  146.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  147.      </item>
  148.      <item>
  149.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12543?af=R</link>
  150.         <pubDate>Tue, 02 Apr 2024 17:04:00 -0700</pubDate>
  151.         <dc:date>2024-04-02T05:04:00-07:00</dc:date>
  152.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  153.         <prism:coverDate/>
  154.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  155.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12543</guid>
  156.         <title>Anxiety and depression in young adults: The role of perceived romantic partner drinking</title>
  157.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  158.         <dc:description>
  159. Abstract
  160. This cross‐sectional study examined how perceived partner drinking relates to relationship satisfaction and symptoms of anxiety and depression across romantic drinking partnerships. Participants included 239 cisgender, heterosexual undergraduate students in current romantic relationships (Mage = 19.74; 76.15% female; 87.87% White) who reported their own and their romantic partner's drinking. Associations between drinking partnerships, identified via K‐medoid cluster analysis, and internalizing symptomatology were examined utilizing hierarchical linear regression. Perceived partner drinking problems were associated with symptoms of depression but not anxiety. Two drinking quantity (concordant light, concordant heavy) and three drinking problem (concordant light, discordant female high, discordant male high) partnerships were identified. Broadly, men's anxiety and relationship satisfaction were significantly impacted by their drinking quantity and problem partnerships, respectively; this effect was not observed for women. The findings of this study extend extant research on the impact of romantic partner drinking to unmarried, collegiate emerging adults who are at an increased risk of internalizing symptomatology and alcohol use and inform conceptualization of drinking partnerships at the drinking quantity and drinking problem level.
  161. </dc:description>
  162.         <content:encoded>
  163. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  164. &lt;p&gt;This cross-sectional study examined how perceived partner drinking relates to relationship satisfaction and symptoms of anxiety and depression across romantic drinking partnerships. Participants included 239 cisgender, heterosexual undergraduate students in current romantic relationships (&lt;i&gt;M&lt;/i&gt;age = 19.74; 76.15% female; 87.87% White) who reported their own and their romantic partner's drinking. Associations between drinking partnerships, identified via K-medoid cluster analysis, and internalizing symptomatology were examined utilizing hierarchical linear regression. Perceived partner drinking problems were associated with symptoms of depression but not anxiety. Two drinking quantity (concordant light, concordant heavy) and three drinking problem (concordant light, discordant female high, discordant male high) partnerships were identified. Broadly, men's anxiety and relationship satisfaction were significantly impacted by their drinking quantity and problem partnerships, respectively; this effect was not observed for women. The findings of this study extend extant research on the impact of romantic partner drinking to unmarried, collegiate emerging adults who are at an increased risk of internalizing symptomatology and alcohol use and inform conceptualization of drinking partnerships at the drinking quantity and drinking problem level.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  165.         <dc:creator>
  166. Katie P. Himes,
  167. Sarah E. Victor,
  168. Adam T. Schmidt,
  169. Andrew K. Littlefield
  170. </dc:creator>
  171.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  172.         <dc:title>Anxiety and depression in young adults: The role of perceived romantic partner drinking</dc:title>
  173.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12543</dc:identifier>
  174.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  175.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12543</prism:doi>
  176.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12543?af=R</prism:url>
  177.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  178.      </item>
  179.      <item>
  180.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12542?af=R</link>
  181.         <pubDate>Tue, 02 Apr 2024 17:00:06 -0700</pubDate>
  182.         <dc:date>2024-04-02T05:00:06-07:00</dc:date>
  183.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  184.         <prism:coverDate/>
  185.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  186.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12542</guid>
  187.         <title>Maintaining social trust: Family cohesion and enhancing mindset in the face of COVID‐19 stress</title>
  188.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  189.         <dc:description>
  190. Abstract
  191. We recruited a large cross‐sectional sample of undergraduate students in China (n = 1134) early in the COVID‐19 pandemic to investigate how family cohesion may be related to the maintenance of social trust. Family cohesion was positively associated with social trust, and this effect was partially mediated by psychological stress responses to COVID‐19 (PSRC), that is, family cohesion was negatively associated with greater PSRC which was also negatively associated with social trust. Having a “stress‐as‐enhancing” mindset had an additive negative effect on the association between family cohesion and PSRC but only had a weak dampening effect on the negative association between PSRC and trust. Interventions focusing on cultivating positive family relationships and embracing a stress‐as‐enhancing mindset may offer avenues for maximizing protective factors in times of public health crises.
  192. </dc:description>
  193.         <content:encoded>
  194. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  195. &lt;p&gt;We recruited a large cross-sectional sample of undergraduate students in China (&lt;i&gt;n&lt;/i&gt; = 1134) early in the COVID-19 pandemic to investigate how family cohesion may be related to the maintenance of social trust. Family cohesion was positively associated with social trust, and this effect was partially mediated by psychological stress responses to COVID-19 (PSRC), that is, family cohesion was negatively associated with greater PSRC which was also negatively associated with social trust. Having a “&lt;i&gt;stress-as-enhancing&lt;/i&gt;” mindset had an additive negative effect on the association between family cohesion and PSRC but only had a weak dampening effect on the negative association between PSRC and trust. Interventions focusing on cultivating positive family relationships and embracing a stress-as-enhancing mindset may offer avenues for maximizing protective factors in times of public health crises.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  196.         <dc:creator>
  197. Baojuan Ye,
  198. Zixuan Zhu,
  199. Hohjin Im,
  200. Xun Chen,
  201. Nannan Fan,
  202. Qiang Yang,
  203. Fei Xia
  204. </dc:creator>
  205.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  206.         <dc:title>Maintaining social trust: Family cohesion and enhancing mindset in the face of COVID‐19 stress</dc:title>
  207.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12542</dc:identifier>
  208.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  209.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12542</prism:doi>
  210.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12542?af=R</prism:url>
  211.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  212.      </item>
  213.      <item>
  214.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12539?af=R</link>
  215.         <pubDate>Wed, 06 Mar 2024 00:09:42 -0800</pubDate>
  216.         <dc:date>2024-03-06T12:09:42-08:00</dc:date>
  217.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  218.         <prism:coverDate/>
  219.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  220.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12539</guid>
  221.         <title>How couples think about money: Types of money motives and relationship satisfaction</title>
  222.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  223.         <dc:description>
  224. Abstract
  225. Two studies examined how financial values held by romantic partners were linked with relationship satisfaction. Across a sample of married individuals (N = 628), and a dyadic sample (N = 236), results suggest that holding or perceiving a romantic partner to hold integrated money motives—wanting to earn money to feel pride, establish one's worth, facilitate freedom, and enrich leisure activities—was linked with better relationship satisfaction. Holding or perceiving a romantic partner to hold nonintegrated money motives—wanting to earn money to enable impulse spending, to feel better than others, and to overcome self‐doubt—was linked with worse relationship satisfaction. In both samples, perceived similarity in money motives between the self and the partner was also linked to higher relationship satisfaction. Study 2 further showed that actual similarity between partners in nonintegrated money motives was also linked to better relationship satisfaction, suggesting that even nonintegrated money motives might benefit relationships, as long as both partners share these motives. Overall, these studies suggest that while holding similar financial values as your partner is linked with better relationships, some financial values are more conducive to relationship satisfaction than others.
  226. </dc:description>
  227.         <content:encoded>
  228. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  229. &lt;p&gt;Two studies examined how financial values held by romantic partners were linked with relationship satisfaction. Across a sample of married individuals (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 628), and a dyadic sample (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 236), results suggest that holding or perceiving a romantic partner to hold &lt;i&gt;integrated&lt;/i&gt; money motives—wanting to earn money to feel pride, establish one's worth, facilitate freedom, and enrich leisure activities—was linked with better relationship satisfaction. Holding or perceiving a romantic partner to hold &lt;i&gt;nonintegrated&lt;/i&gt; money motives—wanting to earn money to enable impulse spending, to feel better than others, and to overcome self-doubt—was linked with worse relationship satisfaction. In both samples, perceived similarity in money motives between the self and the partner was also linked to higher relationship satisfaction. Study 2 further showed that actual similarity between partners in nonintegrated money motives was also linked to better relationship satisfaction, suggesting that even nonintegrated money motives might benefit relationships, as long as both partners share these motives. Overall, these studies suggest that while holding similar financial values as your partner is linked with better relationships, some financial values are more conducive to relationship satisfaction than others.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  230.         <dc:creator>
  231. Johanna Peetz,
  232. Morgan Joseph
  233. </dc:creator>
  234.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  235.         <dc:title>How couples think about money: Types of money motives and relationship satisfaction</dc:title>
  236.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12539</dc:identifier>
  237.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  238.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12539</prism:doi>
  239.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12539?af=R</prism:url>
  240.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  241.      </item>
  242.      <item>
  243.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12500?af=R</link>
  244.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  245.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  246.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  247.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  248.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  249.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12500</guid>
  250.         <title>Issue Information</title>
  251.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 1-3, March 2024. </description>
  252.         <dc:description/>
  253.         <content:encoded/>
  254.         <dc:creator/>
  255.         <category>ISSUE INFORMATION</category>
  256.         <dc:title>Issue Information</dc:title>
  257.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12500</dc:identifier>
  258.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  259.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12500</prism:doi>
  260.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12500?af=R</prism:url>
  261.         <prism:section>ISSUE INFORMATION</prism:section>
  262.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  263.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  264.      </item>
  265.      <item>
  266.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12523?af=R</link>
  267.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  268.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  269.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  270.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  271.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  272.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12523</guid>
  273.         <title>Desperate or desirable? Perceptions of individuals seeking dates online and offline</title>
  274.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 78-90, March 2024. </description>
  275.         <dc:description>
  276. Abstract
  277. Past research suggests that people who use the Internet to pursue romantic relationships have been stereotyped negatively—as unattractive, desperate, or creepy. It is possible, however, that as finding dates online has grown in popularity, individuals who have themselves used online methods to meet a partner are less likely to apply these negative stereotypes than non‐users. In addition, as options for dating online have proliferated, it is not clear that users of all online formats are viewed negatively, or how perceptions of users of online methods might differ from perceptions of daters using various offline methods. This study examined perceptions of those who use various online (algorithm‐based, profile‐browsing, or social media) and offline (meeting through family/friends, luck, groups, work, or going out) methods to meet a partner. Participants (N = 214), who were themselves users or non‐users of online methods of meeting partners, were recruited through Amazon's Mechanical Turk system to complete online questionnaires. Results indicated that participants viewed individuals who used online methods more negatively than those using offline methods; however, individuals who had themselves used online methods viewed other online users more positively than did non‐users.
  278. </dc:description>
  279.         <content:encoded>
  280. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  281. &lt;p&gt;Past research suggests that people who use the Internet to pursue romantic relationships have been stereotyped negatively—as unattractive, desperate, or creepy. It is possible, however, that as finding dates online has grown in popularity, individuals who have themselves used online methods to meet a partner are less likely to apply these negative stereotypes than non-users. In addition, as options for dating online have proliferated, it is not clear that users of all online formats are viewed negatively, or how perceptions of users of online methods might differ from perceptions of daters using various offline methods. This study examined perceptions of those who use various online (algorithm-based, profile-browsing, or social media) and offline (meeting through family/friends, luck, groups, work, or going out) methods to meet a partner. Participants (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 214), who were themselves users or non-users of online methods of meeting partners, were recruited through Amazon's Mechanical Turk system to complete online questionnaires. Results indicated that participants viewed individuals who used online methods more negatively than those using offline methods; however, individuals who had themselves used online methods viewed other online users more positively than did non-users.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  282.         <dc:creator>
  283. Trenton C. Johanis,
  284. Claire E. Midgley,
  285. Penelope Lockwood
  286. </dc:creator>
  287.         <category>BRIEF REPORT</category>
  288.         <dc:title>Desperate or desirable? Perceptions of individuals seeking dates online and offline</dc:title>
  289.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12523</dc:identifier>
  290.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  291.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12523</prism:doi>
  292.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12523?af=R</prism:url>
  293.         <prism:section>BRIEF REPORT</prism:section>
  294.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  295.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  296.      </item>
  297.      <item>
  298.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12534?af=R</link>
  299.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  300.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  301.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  302.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  303.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  304.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12534</guid>
  305.         <title>Latinx young adults' retrospective sibling caregiving: Associations with ethnic identity, responsibility, and depressive symptoms</title>
  306.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 67-77, March 2024. </description>
  307.         <dc:description>
  308. Abstract
  309. The current research used reports among 350 Latinx young adults (60.3% male) between 18 and 21 years (Mage = 20.18, SD = 1.02). Using the cultural transmission model (Carlo &amp; de Guzman, Handbook of U.S. Latino psychology. Sage Publications, Inc., 2009), the goal of the current study was to investigate the associations that sibling caregiving during adolescence, measured retrospectively, may have on ethnic identity resolution (EIR), and in turn, depressive symptoms, and responsibility among Latinx young adults. Path analysis results suggest that sibling caregiving was positively associated with EIR and responsibility. EIR was negatively associated with depressive symptoms and positively associated with responsibility. Both indirect effects via EIR were significant: sibling caregiving to depressive symptoms and responsibility. Our findings highlight that sibling caregiving affects young adult outcomes indirectly via ethnic identity development. The findings have important implications for practitioners and researchers to understand how ethnic identity may be related to young adult outcomes through a sibling‐focused lens. Family interventions that leverage sibling relationships can be incorporated to improve the well‐being of Latinx adolescents and young adults.
  310. </dc:description>
  311.         <content:encoded>
  312. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  313. &lt;p&gt;The current research used reports among 350 Latinx young adults (60.3% male) between 18 and 21 years (&lt;i&gt;Mage&lt;/i&gt; = 20.18, SD = 1.02). Using the cultural transmission model (Carlo &amp;amp; de Guzman, &lt;i&gt;Handbook of U.S. Latino psychology&lt;/i&gt;. Sage Publications, Inc., 2009), the goal of the current study was to investigate the associations that sibling caregiving during adolescence, measured retrospectively, may have on ethnic identity resolution (EIR), and in turn, depressive symptoms, and responsibility among Latinx young adults. Path analysis results suggest that sibling caregiving was positively associated with EIR and responsibility. EIR was negatively associated with depressive symptoms and positively associated with responsibility. Both indirect effects via EIR were significant: sibling caregiving to depressive symptoms and responsibility. Our findings highlight that sibling caregiving affects young adult outcomes indirectly via ethnic identity development. The findings have important implications for practitioners and researchers to understand how ethnic identity may be related to young adult outcomes through a sibling-focused lens. Family interventions that leverage sibling relationships can be incorporated to improve the well-being of Latinx adolescents and young adults.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  314.         <dc:creator>
  315. Gabrielle Kline,
  316. Sahitya Maiya,
  317. Fiorella L. Carlos Chavez
  318. </dc:creator>
  319.         <category>BRIEF REPORT</category>
  320.         <dc:title>Latinx young adults' retrospective sibling caregiving: Associations with ethnic identity, responsibility, and depressive symptoms</dc:title>
  321.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12534</dc:identifier>
  322.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  323.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12534</prism:doi>
  324.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12534?af=R</prism:url>
  325.         <prism:section>BRIEF REPORT</prism:section>
  326.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  327.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  328.      </item>
  329.      <item>
  330.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12522?af=R</link>
  331.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  332.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  333.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  334.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  335.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  336.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12522</guid>
  337.         <title>Development and empirical test of the research‐informed South African Relationship Functioning Assessment (SARFA)</title>
  338.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 44-66, March 2024. </description>
  339.         <dc:description>
  340. Abstract
  341. Intimate partners play an important role in chronic diseases. Despite the chronic disease burden in sub‐Saharan Africa, very few culturally relevant quantitative measures of intimate relationship functioning are available. We conducted an empirical investigation evaluating the psychometric properties of the South African Relationship Functioning Assessment (SARFA) assessing healthy relationship functioning in N = 150 community members (50% women; M age = 27.2 years) living in the Vulindlela area of KwaZulu‐Natal, South Africa. Item development was based on prior qualitative research from two South African communities. All assessments were conducted in isiZulu, participants' primary language. An exploratory factor analysis was conducted on the initial 39‐item measure. The best‐fitting model consisted of one factor with 22 items. The SARFA's internal consistency was α = .94. Convergent validity was observed via significant positive associations (all rs ≥ .38, p &lt; .001) between the SARFA's total score and measures of trust, emotional intimacy, constructive communication, sexual satisfaction, and relationship control (women only). Divergent validity was observed for women only. Encouraging initial psychometric properties of a culturally relevant measure of relationship functioning in KwaZulu‐Natal may have relevance to other communities and potential to be used in research involving couples and health in chronic disease‐burdened communities.
  342. </dc:description>
  343.         <content:encoded>
  344. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  345. &lt;p&gt;Intimate partners play an important role in chronic diseases. Despite the chronic disease burden in sub-Saharan Africa, very few culturally relevant quantitative measures of intimate relationship functioning are available. We conducted an empirical investigation evaluating the psychometric properties of the South African Relationship Functioning Assessment (SARFA) assessing healthy relationship functioning in &lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 150 community members (50% women; &lt;i&gt;M&lt;/i&gt; age = 27.2 years) living in the Vulindlela area of KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Item development was based on prior qualitative research from two South African communities. All assessments were conducted in isiZulu, participants' primary language. An exploratory factor analysis was conducted on the initial 39-item measure. The best-fitting model consisted of one factor with 22 items. The SARFA's internal consistency was &lt;i&gt;α&lt;/i&gt; = .94. Convergent validity was observed via significant positive associations (all &lt;i&gt;r&lt;/i&gt;s ≥ .38, &lt;i&gt;p&lt;/i&gt; &amp;lt; .001) between the SARFA's total score and measures of trust, emotional intimacy, constructive communication, sexual satisfaction, and relationship control (women only). Divergent validity was observed for women only. Encouraging initial psychometric properties of a culturally relevant measure of relationship functioning in KwaZulu-Natal may have relevance to other communities and potential to be used in research involving couples and health in chronic disease-burdened communities.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  346.         <dc:creator>
  347. Jennifer M. Belus,
  348. Abigail C. Hines,
  349. Jessica F. Magidson,
  350. Derek K. Iwamoto,
  351. Alexandra L. Rose,
  352. Alison Li,
  353. Ruanne V. Barnabas,
  354. Alastair van Heerden
  355. </dc:creator>
  356.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  357.         <dc:title>Development and empirical test of the research‐informed South African Relationship Functioning Assessment (SARFA)</dc:title>
  358.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12522</dc:identifier>
  359.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  360.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12522</prism:doi>
  361.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12522?af=R</prism:url>
  362.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  363.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  364.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  365.      </item>
  366.      <item>
  367.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12525?af=R</link>
  368.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  369.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  370.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  371.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  372.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  373.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12525</guid>
  374.         <title>Exploring linguistic markers, threat appraisals, and genetic variation during mixed‐sex couples' conflict conversations</title>
  375.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 175-189, March 2024. </description>
  376.         <dc:description>
  377. Abstract
  378. How couples communicate during conflict interactions can have important implications for their relational well‐being. The theory of resilience and relational load provides a valuable lens for exploring the associations among couples' language use during conflict conversations and the extent to which such conversations are perceived as threatening to one's relationship, as well as whether genetic markers moderate such associations. Forty‐seven mixed‐sex couples had a conversation about a topic causing conflict in their relationship and provided saliva samples that were tested for their genotype (GG, AA, or AG) for oxytocin receptor gene rs53576. The results revealed that neither the use of communal language nor positive emotion words were associated with perceiving the conversation as less threatening to their relationship. Genetic variation did not moderate the associations with either positive emotion word use or communal language. However, a three‐way interaction between sex, OXTR, and positive emotion word use emerged in the post hoc analyses, indicating that positive emotion word use was negatively associated with threat appraisals for women with a GG genotype, and positively associated with threat appraisals for men with a GG genotype. Implications for mixed‐sex couples' communication and research on language use during conflict conversations are discussed.
  379. </dc:description>
  380.         <content:encoded>
  381. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  382. &lt;p&gt;How couples communicate during conflict interactions can have important implications for their relational well-being. The theory of resilience and relational load provides a valuable lens for exploring the associations among couples' language use during conflict conversations and the extent to which such conversations are perceived as threatening to one's relationship, as well as whether genetic markers moderate such associations. Forty-seven mixed-sex couples had a conversation about a topic causing conflict in their relationship and provided saliva samples that were tested for their genotype (GG, AA, or AG) for oxytocin receptor gene rs53576. The results revealed that neither the use of communal language nor positive emotion words were associated with perceiving the conversation as less threatening to their relationship. Genetic variation did not moderate the associations with either positive emotion word use or communal language. However, a three-way interaction between sex, OXTR, and positive emotion word use emerged in the post hoc analyses, indicating that positive emotion word use was negatively associated with threat appraisals for women with a GG genotype, and positively associated with threat appraisals for men with a GG genotype. Implications for mixed-sex couples' communication and research on language use during conflict conversations are discussed.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  383.         <dc:creator>
  384. Amanda Denes,
  385. John P. Crowley,
  386. Jessica Gasiorek,
  387. Anuraj Dhillon,
  388. Margaret Bennett‐Brown,
  389. Kara L. Winkler,
  390. Ambyre L. P. Ponivas
  391. </dc:creator>
  392.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  393.         <dc:title>Exploring linguistic markers, threat appraisals, and genetic variation during mixed‐sex couples' conflict conversations</dc:title>
  394.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12525</dc:identifier>
  395.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  396.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12525</prism:doi>
  397.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12525?af=R</prism:url>
  398.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  399.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  400.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  401.      </item>
  402.      <item>
  403.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12526?af=R</link>
  404.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  405.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  406.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  407.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  408.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  409.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12526</guid>
  410.         <title>An examination of specialness meaning framework threat in reactive and retroactive romantic jealousy experiences</title>
  411.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 190-211, March 2024. </description>
  412.         <dc:description>
  413. Abstract
  414. Extant jealousy models explain cases of reactive jealousy, which occurs in response to a partner's unambiguous romantic or sexual involvement with a rival from the present. However, they likely cannot explain cases of retroactive romantic jealousy, which is evoked in response to information about a partner's previous romantic or sexual experiences that occurred before the primary relationship began. Some prior theoretical speculation and research suggest that a sense of specialness may be threatened in jealousy‐evoking situations involving either a partner's current or past romantic activities. The idea that jealousy stems from a threat to expectations of specialness is consistent with a broader model of threat, the meaning maintenance model (MMM). The main purpose of this study was to experimentally test MMM predictions in the context of both reactive and retroactive jealousy alongside of predictions derived from extant jealousy models. The study examined the role of attachment as well. Results indicate expectations of specialness were threatened in experiences of both types of jealousy, and a threat to specialness was the only type of threat perceived for retroactive jealousy. The results also help clarify previous findings regarding attachment and romantic jealousy.
  415. </dc:description>
  416.         <content:encoded>
  417. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  418. &lt;p&gt;Extant jealousy models explain cases of &lt;i&gt;reactive jealousy&lt;/i&gt;, which occurs in response to a partner's unambiguous romantic or sexual involvement with a rival from the present. However, they likely cannot explain cases of &lt;i&gt;retroactive romantic jealousy&lt;/i&gt;, which is evoked in response to information about a partner's previous romantic or sexual experiences that occurred before the primary relationship began. Some prior theoretical speculation and research suggest that a sense of specialness may be threatened in jealousy-evoking situations involving either a partner's current or past romantic activities. The idea that jealousy stems from a threat to expectations of specialness is consistent with a broader model of threat, the meaning maintenance model (MMM). The main purpose of this study was to experimentally test MMM predictions in the context of both reactive and retroactive jealousy alongside of predictions derived from extant jealousy models. The study examined the role of attachment as well. Results indicate expectations of specialness were threatened in experiences of both types of jealousy, and a threat to specialness was the only type of threat perceived for retroactive jealousy. The results also help clarify previous findings regarding attachment and romantic jealousy.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  419.         <dc:creator>
  420. Jessica R. Frampton
  421. </dc:creator>
  422.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  423.         <dc:title>An examination of specialness meaning framework threat in reactive and retroactive romantic jealousy experiences</dc:title>
  424.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12526</dc:identifier>
  425.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  426.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12526</prism:doi>
  427.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12526?af=R</prism:url>
  428.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  429.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  430.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  431.      </item>
  432.      <item>
  433.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12530?af=R</link>
  434.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  435.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  436.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  437.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  438.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  439.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12530</guid>
  440.         <title>Are we there for each other? Longitudinal associations between parenting stress and coparenting in parents of preschool and school‐aged children</title>
  441.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 259-277, March 2024. </description>
  442.         <dc:description>
  443. Abstract
  444. It is well known that parenthood can be particularly stressful. However, less is known about the stability of parenting stress across children's developmental periods. Certain correlates to parenting stress, such as coparenting support between parents, also appear to play a crucial role in childrearing. The current study aims to shed light on the longitudinal associations between parenting stress during the preschool and school years, along with the moderating effect of coparenting support in this association. Eighty‐two heterosexual couples who are parents completed the Parenting Stress Index at Time 1 and Time 2 and the Coparenting Relationship Scale at Time 2. Actor‐Partner path analyses revealed that greater parenting stress in each parent was related to their partner's greater parenting stress at each time point, but only to their own greater parenting stress 5 years later. The association between fathers' parenting stress at both time points was weaker in fathers who reported greater coparenting support from their partner. Helping parents reduce their parenting stress and learn to support each other effectively as coparents may be important parenting intervention avenues.
  445. </dc:description>
  446.         <content:encoded>
  447. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  448. &lt;p&gt;It is well known that parenthood can be particularly stressful. However, less is known about the stability of parenting stress across children's developmental periods. Certain correlates to parenting stress, such as coparenting support between parents, also appear to play a crucial role in childrearing. The current study aims to shed light on the longitudinal associations between parenting stress during the preschool and school years, along with the moderating effect of coparenting support in this association. Eighty-two heterosexual couples who are parents completed the Parenting Stress Index at Time 1 and Time 2 and the Coparenting Relationship Scale at Time 2. Actor-Partner path analyses revealed that greater parenting stress in each parent was related to their partner's greater parenting stress at each time point, but only to their own greater parenting stress 5 years later. The association between fathers' parenting stress at both time points was weaker in fathers who reported greater coparenting support from their partner. Helping parents reduce their parenting stress and learn to support each other effectively as coparents may be important parenting intervention avenues.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  449.         <dc:creator>
  450. Stéphanie Azzi,
  451. Marie‐France Lafontaine,
  452. Jean‐François Bureau,
  453. Audrey Brassard
  454. </dc:creator>
  455.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  456.         <dc:title>Are we there for each other? Longitudinal associations between parenting stress and coparenting in parents of preschool and school‐aged children</dc:title>
  457.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12530</dc:identifier>
  458.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  459.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12530</prism:doi>
  460.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12530?af=R</prism:url>
  461.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  462.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  463.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  464.      </item>
  465.      <item>
  466.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12531?af=R</link>
  467.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  468.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  469.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  470.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  471.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  472.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12531</guid>
  473.         <title>Distal sibling grief: Exploring emotional affect and salience of listener behaviors in stories of sibling death</title>
  474.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 112-131, March 2024. </description>
  475.         <dc:description>
  476. Abstract
  477. Communicated Narrative Sense‐Making (CNSM) theory was utilized to consider the characteristics of stories of sibling death 5 years or more after the loss and to investigate the association between sharing these stories and a bereaved sibling's well‐being. Analysis of 174 narratives revealed that a neutral affective tone was predominant in storytelling and that participant overall mood emerged as a more significant predictor of well‐being than the affective content of the story. Cause of death, age at death, short‐ and long‐term impacts, and continuing bonds with sibling were the main themes included in sibling death stories, with an emphasis on factual content. The participants' reported salience of six CNSM perspective‐taking behaviors of listeners while telling the death story to others suggests that while all six behaviors were important to their decision to share their story, being offered the freedom to tell the story and having positive, attentive behaviors from listeners were the most salient. Based on these findings, contributions to CNSM translational interventions for bereaved siblings are discussed.
  478. </dc:description>
  479.         <content:encoded>
  480. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  481. &lt;p&gt;Communicated Narrative Sense-Making (CNSM) theory was utilized to consider the characteristics of stories of sibling death 5 years or more after the loss and to investigate the association between sharing these stories and a bereaved sibling's well-being. Analysis of 174 narratives revealed that a neutral affective tone was predominant in storytelling and that participant overall mood emerged as a more significant predictor of well-being than the affective content of the story. Cause of death, age at death, short- and long-term impacts, and continuing bonds with sibling were the main themes included in sibling death stories, with an emphasis on factual content. The participants' reported salience of six CNSM perspective-taking behaviors of listeners while telling the death story to others suggests that while all six behaviors were important to their decision to share their story, being offered the freedom to tell the story and having positive, attentive behaviors from listeners were the most salient. Based on these findings, contributions to CNSM translational interventions for bereaved siblings are discussed.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  482.         <dc:creator>
  483. Margaret Brock,
  484. Christina Granato Yoshimura
  485. </dc:creator>
  486.         <category>MIXED METHODS</category>
  487.         <dc:title>Distal sibling grief: Exploring emotional affect and salience of listener behaviors in stories of sibling death</dc:title>
  488.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12531</dc:identifier>
  489.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  490.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12531</prism:doi>
  491.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12531?af=R</prism:url>
  492.         <prism:section>MIXED METHODS</prism:section>
  493.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  494.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  495.      </item>
  496.      <item>
  497.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12533?af=R</link>
  498.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  499.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  500.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  501.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  502.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  503.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12533</guid>
  504.         <title>Re‐evaluating the honing framework: Naturalistic observation of same‐ and different‐sex couples' conversations</title>
  505.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 156-174, March 2024. </description>
  506.         <dc:description>
  507. Abstract
  508. The current study tested the honing framework, which posits that people in same‐, versus different‐, sex couples may reduce their social networks to primarily include members perceived as supportive, facilitating more satisfying social interactions and enjoying more positive affect. The honing framework also predicts similarities among people in same‐ and different‐sex couples, including quantity of social interactions, and social interaction links to well‐being. Seventy‐eight couples participated: 25 women with women, 19 men with men, and 34 different‐sex couples. Over two weekends, both partners wore the Electronically Activated Recorder, a device that records audible, naturally‐occurring social interactions. Additionally, each partner completed well‐being questionnaires. Actor‐partner interdependence models mostly supported the honing framework, revealing similarities among same‐ and different‐sex couples, including the quantity of social interactions, as well as interaction quantity links to well‐being. The data also revealed a novel, unpredicted component of the honing framework: people in same‐sex couples tended to have more one‐on‐one and fewer group interactions compared to people in different‐sex couples. This lends support to and further develops the honing framework, revealing that people in same‐sex couples manage to hone their social networks to close, supportive members with whom they have one‐on‐one, satisfying interactions.
  509. </dc:description>
  510.         <content:encoded>
  511. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  512. &lt;p&gt;The current study tested the honing framework, which posits that people in same-, versus different-, sex couples may reduce their social networks to primarily include members perceived as supportive, facilitating more satisfying social interactions and enjoying more positive affect. The honing framework also predicts similarities among people in same- and different-sex couples, including quantity of social interactions, and social interaction links to well-being. Seventy-eight couples participated: 25 women with women, 19 men with men, and 34 different-sex couples. Over two weekends, both partners wore the Electronically Activated Recorder, a device that records audible, naturally-occurring social interactions. Additionally, each partner completed well-being questionnaires. Actor-partner interdependence models mostly supported the honing framework, revealing similarities among same- and different-sex couples, including the quantity of social interactions, as well as interaction quantity links to well-being. The data also revealed a novel, unpredicted component of the honing framework: people in same-sex couples tended to have more one-on-one and fewer group interactions compared to people in different-sex couples. This lends support to and further develops the honing framework, revealing that people in same-sex couples manage to hone their social networks to close, supportive members with whom they have one-on-one, satisfying interactions.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  513.         <dc:creator>
  514. Megan L. Robbins,
  515. Chandler M. Spahr,
  516. Alexander Karan
  517. </dc:creator>
  518.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  519.         <dc:title>Re‐evaluating the honing framework: Naturalistic observation of same‐ and different‐sex couples' conversations</dc:title>
  520.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12533</dc:identifier>
  521.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  522.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12533</prism:doi>
  523.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12533?af=R</prism:url>
  524.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  525.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  526.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  527.      </item>
  528.      <item>
  529.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12521?af=R</link>
  530.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  531.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  532.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  533.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  534.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  535.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12521</guid>
  536.         <title>Strategies for becoming a more desirable mate: Evidence from 14 countries</title>
  537.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 4-23, March 2024. </description>
  538.         <dc:description>
  539. Abstract
  540. The current research aimed to study the strategies that people employ in order to become more desirable as mates in different cultural settings. More specifically, using a closed‐ended questionnaire on a sample of 7181 participants from 14 different countries, we identified 10 different strategies that people employ to become more appealing as mates. Participants indicated that they had more frequently used the “Enhance looks,” followed by the “Show off abilities and talents,” and the “Demonstrate similarity” strategies. On the other hand, they had less frequently used the “Keep undesirable things hidden,” the “Show off and exaggerate wealth and abilities,” and the “Drastic appearance changes” strategies. Female participants indicated that they had more extensive used the “Enhance looks” strategy than male participants, while male participants indicated that they had more extensive used the “Increase income and social status” and the “Show off and exaggerate wealth and abilities” strategies than female participants. The sex effects, as well as the extent of use, were generally consistent across the different cultures. The identified strategies were classified further into two main strategies, namely the “Develop and demonstrate desirable traits” and the “Deceive about undesirable traits,” which was generally consistent across the different countries.
  541. </dc:description>
  542.         <content:encoded>
  543. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  544. &lt;p&gt;The current research aimed to study the strategies that people employ in order to become more desirable as mates in different cultural settings. More specifically, using a closed-ended questionnaire on a sample of 7181 participants from 14 different countries, we identified 10 different strategies that people employ to become more appealing as mates. Participants indicated that they had more frequently used the “Enhance looks,” followed by the “Show off abilities and talents,” and the “Demonstrate similarity” strategies. On the other hand, they had less frequently used the “Keep undesirable things hidden,” the “Show off and exaggerate wealth and abilities,” and the “Drastic appearance changes” strategies. Female participants indicated that they had more extensive used the “Enhance looks” strategy than male participants, while male participants indicated that they had more extensive used the “Increase income and social status” and the “Show off and exaggerate wealth and abilities” strategies than female participants. The sex effects, as well as the extent of use, were generally consistent across the different cultures. The identified strategies were classified further into two main strategies, namely the “Develop and demonstrate desirable traits” and the “Deceive about undesirable traits,” which was generally consistent across the different countries.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  545.         <dc:creator>
  546. Menelaos Apostolou,
  547. Mark Sullman,
  548. Béla Birkás,
  549. Agata Błachnio,
  550. Ekaterina Bushina,
  551. Fran Calvo,
  552. William Costello,
  553. Tanja Dujlovic,
  554. Tetiana Hill,
  555. Yanina Lisun,
  556. Denisse Manrique‐Millones,
  557. Oscar Manrique‐Pino,
  558. Norbert Meskó,
  559. Martin Nechtelberger,
  560. Yohsuke Ohtsubo,
  561. Christian Kenji Ollhoff,
  562. Aneta Przepiórka,
  563. Ádám Putz,
  564. Mariaelena Tagliabue,
  565. Burcu Tekeş,
  566. Andrew Thomas,
  567. Jaroslava Varella Valentova,
  568. Marco Antonio Correa Varella,
  569. Yan Wang,
  570. Paula Wright,
  571. Sílvia Font‐Mayolas
  572. </dc:creator>
  573.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  574.         <dc:title>Strategies for becoming a more desirable mate: Evidence from 14 countries</dc:title>
  575.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12521</dc:identifier>
  576.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  577.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12521</prism:doi>
  578.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12521?af=R</prism:url>
  579.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  580.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  581.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  582.      </item>
  583.      <item>
  584.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12524?af=R</link>
  585.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  586.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  587.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  588.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  589.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  590.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12524</guid>
  591.         <title>Committed to staying single: Adapting the investment model of commitment processes to study singlehood</title>
  592.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 132-155, March 2024. </description>
  593.         <dc:description>
  594. Abstract
  595. In the current work, we adapt the investment model of commitment processes for single adults. Across two cross‐sectional studies using undergraduate and internet samples (total N = 647), we tested whether the same factors that predict relationship commitment (investment, satisfaction, and quality of alternatives) also predict commitment to singlehood. In line with this theoretical model, we found that singlehood satisfaction and investment in singlehood were each positively associated with commitment to singlehood, whereas the quality of alternatives to singlehood was negatively associated with commitment to singlehood. We also found that a person's expected satisfaction and planned investment were each associated with commitment to singlehood, whereas subjective norms were not associated with commitment to singlehood. In addition, we tested whether commitment to singlehood was positively associated with well‐being, but these associations were nonsignificant. Our findings provide initial evidence for the appropriateness of the investment model for understanding commitment to being single.
  596. </dc:description>
  597.         <content:encoded>
  598. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  599. &lt;p&gt;In the current work, we adapt the investment model of commitment processes for single adults. Across two cross-sectional studies using undergraduate and internet samples (total &lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 647), we tested whether the same factors that predict relationship commitment (investment, satisfaction, and quality of alternatives) also predict commitment to singlehood. In line with this theoretical model, we found that singlehood satisfaction and investment in singlehood were each positively associated with commitment to singlehood, whereas the quality of alternatives to singlehood was negatively associated with commitment to singlehood. We also found that a person's expected satisfaction and planned investment were each associated with commitment to singlehood, whereas subjective norms were not associated with commitment to singlehood. In addition, we tested whether commitment to singlehood was positively associated with well-being, but these associations were nonsignificant. Our findings provide initial evidence for the appropriateness of the investment model for understanding commitment to being single.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  600.         <dc:creator>
  601. Emily T. Beauparlant,
  602. Laura V. Machia,
  603. Jeewon Oh
  604. </dc:creator>
  605.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  606.         <dc:title>Committed to staying single: Adapting the investment model of commitment processes to study singlehood</dc:title>
  607.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12524</dc:identifier>
  608.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  609.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12524</prism:doi>
  610.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12524?af=R</prism:url>
  611.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  612.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  613.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  614.      </item>
  615.      <item>
  616.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12527?af=R</link>
  617.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  618.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  619.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  620.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  621.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  622.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12527</guid>
  623.         <title>Just between us…: The role of sharing and receiving secrets in friendship across time</title>
  624.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 91-111, March 2024. </description>
  625.         <dc:description>
  626. Abstract
  627. Secrets are prominent in close relationships. Most research has examined people's general tendencies to share secrets or single instances of secret‐keeping or secret‐sharing. These methods limit what we know about how keeping and sharing secrets across time in established close relationships is associated with important relationship characteristics. This research focused on the role that secret exchanges play in how people assess relational closeness and social utility (i.e., being a valuable social connection) in friendships. Across 10 weeks, participants (N = 126) reported bi‐weekly on sharing and receiving personal and secondhand secrets (i.e., third‐party secrets) with three friends. We found that sharing and receiving more personal secrets were robustly associated with higher closeness and social utility. The findings on secondhand secret‐sharing were more complex, suggesting that receiving secondhand secrets from friends and sharing secondhand secrets with friends were differentially associated with closeness and social utility when comparing short‐term versus cumulative effects. Results are discussed in terms of how secret‐sharing behaviors are dynamically associated with close relationship qualities.
  628. </dc:description>
  629.         <content:encoded>
  630. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  631. &lt;p&gt;Secrets are prominent in close relationships. Most research has examined people's general tendencies to share secrets or single instances of secret-keeping or secret-sharing. These methods limit what we know about how keeping and sharing secrets across time in established close relationships is associated with important relationship characteristics. This research focused on the role that secret exchanges play in how people assess relational closeness and social utility (i.e., being a valuable social connection) in friendships. Across 10 weeks, participants (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 126) reported bi-weekly on sharing and receiving personal and secondhand secrets (i.e., third-party secrets) with three friends. We found that sharing and receiving more personal secrets were robustly associated with higher closeness and social utility. The findings on secondhand secret-sharing were more complex, suggesting that receiving secondhand secrets from friends and sharing secondhand secrets with friends were differentially associated with closeness and social utility when comparing short-term versus cumulative effects. Results are discussed in terms of how secret-sharing behaviors are dynamically associated with close relationship qualities.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  632.         <dc:creator>
  633. Alisa Bedrov,
  634. Shelly L. Gable
  635. </dc:creator>
  636.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  637.         <dc:title>Just between us…: The role of sharing and receiving secrets in friendship across time</dc:title>
  638.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12527</dc:identifier>
  639.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  640.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12527</prism:doi>
  641.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12527?af=R</prism:url>
  642.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  643.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  644.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  645.      </item>
  646.      <item>
  647.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12528?af=R</link>
  648.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  649.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  650.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  651.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  652.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  653.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12528</guid>
  654.         <title>Depressed mood and perception of negative partner behavior in couple interactions: A daily diary study</title>
  655.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 242-258, March 2024. </description>
  656.         <dc:description>
  657. Abstract
  658. Despite extensive research on depression and couple interactions, little is known about how depressed mood influences couples' experience of everyday life interactions. In this study, data were gathered from 72 different‐gender couples (N = 144 individuals), who reported their feelings, behavior, and perceptions of their partner's behavior several times a day over 14 days. The study revealed that when individuals reported feeling more depressed, they perceived their romantic partner's behavior as more distant and hurtful, and they felt treated worse and more rejected. Moreover, when individuals reported feeling more depressed, their romantic partners perceived them as more distant, and they reported feeling treated worse and more rejected. However, depressed mood did not predict subsequent relationship perceptions in time‐lagged associations, and the directionality from relationship perceptions to depressed mood was inconclusive.
  659. </dc:description>
  660.         <content:encoded>
  661. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  662. &lt;p&gt;Despite extensive research on depression and couple interactions, little is known about how depressed mood influences couples' experience of everyday life interactions. In this study, data were gathered from 72 different-gender couples (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 144 individuals), who reported their feelings, behavior, and perceptions of their partner's behavior several times a day over 14 days. The study revealed that when individuals reported feeling more depressed, they perceived their romantic partner's behavior as more distant and hurtful, and they felt treated worse and more rejected. Moreover, when individuals reported feeling more depressed, their romantic partners perceived them as more distant, and they reported feeling treated worse and more rejected. However, depressed mood did not predict subsequent relationship perceptions in time-lagged associations, and the directionality from relationship perceptions to depressed mood was inconclusive.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  663.         <dc:creator>
  664. Tamara Luginbuehl,
  665. Dominik Schoebi,
  666. Pei Hwa Goh,
  667. John V. Miller,
  668. Joanne Davila
  669. </dc:creator>
  670.         <category>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</category>
  671.         <dc:title>Depressed mood and perception of negative partner behavior in couple interactions: A daily diary study</dc:title>
  672.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12528</dc:identifier>
  673.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  674.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12528</prism:doi>
  675.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12528?af=R</prism:url>
  676.         <prism:section>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</prism:section>
  677.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  678.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  679.      </item>
  680.      <item>
  681.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12529?af=R</link>
  682.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  683.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  684.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  685.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  686.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  687.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12529</guid>
  688.         <title>Examining trajectories of marital satisfaction to represent the resilience process among Indonesian married individuals</title>
  689.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 24-43, March 2024. </description>
  690.         <dc:description>
  691. Abstract
  692. Marriage is an important milestone for many adults, and notably in Indonesia, where marriage is also considered a personal accomplishment and social obligation. Research has found being married is associated with greater well‐being, but marriages also face challenges. Resilience, defined as successfully adapting to challenges, is a potential concept to help married individuals maintain or regain adaptation despite challenges in marriages. This is the first relationship study in Indonesia to examine resilience trajectories as represented by marital satisfaction. A weekly repeated measure design was conducted among 135 Indonesian married individuals. Participants reported their experiences of intradyadic and extradyadic stress, and marital satisfaction over 6 weeks. Growth mixture modeling and multinomial logistic regression were used to examine unobserved marital satisfaction trajectories and to estimate the impact of intradyadic and extradyadic stress on trajectory membership. Results suggested three unobserved trajectories: high, moderate, and low levels of marital satisfaction. Higher levels of intradyadic and extradyadic stress increased the probability of belonging to lower satisfaction trajectories. This evidence could be invaluable in helping Indonesian married individuals to better adapt to challenges they face. Future studies can explore protective factors associated with a high satisfaction trajectory to assist married Indonesians in successfully adapting to stress.
  693. </dc:description>
  694.         <content:encoded>
  695. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  696. &lt;p&gt;Marriage is an important milestone for many adults, and notably in Indonesia, where marriage is also considered a personal accomplishment and social obligation. Research has found being married is associated with greater well-being, but marriages also face challenges. Resilience, defined as successfully adapting to challenges, is a potential concept to help married individuals maintain or regain adaptation despite challenges in marriages. This is the first relationship study in Indonesia to examine resilience trajectories as represented by marital satisfaction. A weekly repeated measure design was conducted among 135 Indonesian married individuals. Participants reported their experiences of intradyadic and extradyadic stress, and marital satisfaction over 6 weeks. Growth mixture modeling and multinomial logistic regression were used to examine unobserved marital satisfaction trajectories and to estimate the impact of intradyadic and extradyadic stress on trajectory membership. Results suggested three unobserved trajectories: high, moderate, and low levels of marital satisfaction. Higher levels of intradyadic and extradyadic stress increased the probability of belonging to lower satisfaction trajectories. This evidence could be invaluable in helping Indonesian married individuals to better adapt to challenges they face. Future studies can explore protective factors associated with a high satisfaction trajectory to assist married Indonesians in successfully adapting to stress.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  697.         <dc:creator>
  698. Edwin Adrianta Surijah,
  699. Kate Murray,
  700. Darren Wraith,
  701. Ian Shochet
  702. </dc:creator>
  703.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  704.         <dc:title>Examining trajectories of marital satisfaction to represent the resilience process among Indonesian married individuals</dc:title>
  705.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12529</dc:identifier>
  706.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  707.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12529</prism:doi>
  708.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12529?af=R</prism:url>
  709.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  710.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  711.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  712.      </item>
  713.      <item>
  714.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12532?af=R</link>
  715.         <pubDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 04:24:48 -0800</pubDate>
  716.         <dc:date>2024-03-01T04:24:48-08:00</dc:date>
  717.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  718.         <prism:coverDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDate>
  719.         <prism:coverDisplayDate>Fri, 01 Mar 2024 00:00:00 -0800</prism:coverDisplayDate>
  720.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12532</guid>
  721.         <title>The relative importance of contextual factors in judging mundane extradyadic behaviors as infidelity: A policy‐capturing study</title>
  722.         <description>Personal Relationships, Volume 31, Issue 1, Page 212-241, March 2024. </description>
  723.         <dc:description>
  724. Abstract
  725. Seemingly benign extradyadic behaviors (e.g., buying/receiving gifts or talking on the phone) may be perceived as infidelity under certain circumstances, therefore causing distress and conflict in romantic relationships. A policy‐capturing method was used to illuminate the relative role of contextual factors (secrecy, frequency of the behavior, and the victim's familiarity with the rival) in perceiving whether a mundane, everyday extradyadic act is perceived to cross the line from benign to infidelity. In two sessions, 135 participants completed individual difference measures and rated 30 different vignettes in which the extradyadic behavior (i.e., direct messaging on social media) was held constant, but levels of contextual factors varied. Participants perceived secrecy as the most important contextual factor in deciding whether a mundane extradyadic behavior constituted infidelity, followed by frequency. The victim's familiarity with the rival was deemed least important. Higher reactive jealousy predicted higher perception of the behavior as infidelity and greater anticipated emotional distress following the behavior. Implications for how couples discuss the boundaries of their relationships and understand the source of emotional distress experienced after seemingly benign extradyadic behaviors are discussed.
  726. </dc:description>
  727.         <content:encoded>
  728. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  729. &lt;p&gt;Seemingly benign extradyadic behaviors (e.g., buying/receiving gifts or talking on the phone) may be perceived as infidelity under certain circumstances, therefore causing distress and conflict in romantic relationships. A policy-capturing method was used to illuminate the relative role of contextual factors (secrecy, frequency of the behavior, and the victim's familiarity with the rival) in perceiving whether a mundane, everyday extradyadic act is perceived to cross the line from benign to infidelity. In two sessions, 135 participants completed individual difference measures and rated 30 different vignettes in which the extradyadic behavior (i.e., direct messaging on social media) was held constant, but levels of contextual factors varied. Participants perceived secrecy as the most important contextual factor in deciding whether a mundane extradyadic behavior constituted infidelity, followed by frequency. The victim's familiarity with the rival was deemed least important. Higher reactive jealousy predicted higher perception of the behavior as infidelity and greater anticipated emotional distress following the behavior. Implications for how couples discuss the boundaries of their relationships and understand the source of emotional distress experienced after seemingly benign extradyadic behaviors are discussed.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  730.         <dc:creator>
  731. Sara Salavati,
  732. Susan D. Boon
  733. </dc:creator>
  734.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  735.         <dc:title>The relative importance of contextual factors in judging mundane extradyadic behaviors as infidelity: A policy‐capturing study</dc:title>
  736.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12532</dc:identifier>
  737.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  738.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12532</prism:doi>
  739.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12532?af=R</prism:url>
  740.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  741.         <prism:volume>31</prism:volume>
  742.         <prism:number>1</prism:number>
  743.      </item>
  744.      <item>
  745.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12540?af=R</link>
  746.         <pubDate>Wed, 21 Feb 2024 14:48:41 -0800</pubDate>
  747.         <dc:date>2024-02-21T02:48:41-08:00</dc:date>
  748.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  749.         <prism:coverDate/>
  750.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  751.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12540</guid>
  752.         <title>Heterosexual men's reactions to infidelity revisited: Comparing the sex role presentation of extradyadic female partners</title>
  753.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  754.         <dc:description>
  755. Abstract
  756. Heterosexual men report less distress at infidelity from female interlopers than male interlopers. In addition to presenting no risk of cuckoldry, men could also perceive these women as additional sexual opportunities if they assume mutual interest from the female interloper. The current study considered this possibility by experimentally manipulating the sex role assumption of a female interloper (either masculine or feminine presentation) for expected sexual interest in men. Heterosexual men reacted to infidelity from hypothetical long‐term romantic partners in two experiments. The extrapair mate was another man, a feminine woman, or a masculine‐presenting woman. Although infidelity elicited an overall negative reaction, men reported less distress when the extradyadic partner was female. The feminine woman was additionally regarded as affording the most sexual opportunity. Effects were particularly amplified when this information included images of the extradyadic partner. Studies provide evidence for complementary hypotheses based on intrasexual conflict and intersexual opportunities.
  757. </dc:description>
  758.         <content:encoded>
  759. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  760. &lt;p&gt;Heterosexual men report less distress at infidelity from female interlopers than male interlopers. In addition to presenting no risk of cuckoldry, men could also perceive these women as additional sexual opportunities if they assume mutual interest from the female interloper. The current study considered this possibility by experimentally manipulating the sex role assumption of a female interloper (either masculine or feminine presentation) for expected sexual interest in men. Heterosexual men reacted to infidelity from hypothetical long-term romantic partners in two experiments. The extrapair mate was another man, a feminine woman, or a masculine-presenting woman. Although infidelity elicited an overall negative reaction, men reported less distress when the extradyadic partner was female. The feminine woman was additionally regarded as affording the most sexual opportunity. Effects were particularly amplified when this information included images of the extradyadic partner. Studies provide evidence for complementary hypotheses based on intrasexual conflict and intersexual opportunities.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  761.         <dc:creator>
  762. Mitch Brown,
  763. Samuel E. Snowden,
  764. Seth M. Bridges
  765. </dc:creator>
  766.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  767.         <dc:title>Heterosexual men's reactions to infidelity revisited: Comparing the sex role presentation of extradyadic female partners</dc:title>
  768.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12540</dc:identifier>
  769.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  770.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12540</prism:doi>
  771.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12540?af=R</prism:url>
  772.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  773.      </item>
  774.      <item>
  775.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12538?af=R</link>
  776.         <pubDate>Tue, 13 Feb 2024 14:18:31 -0800</pubDate>
  777.         <dc:date>2024-02-13T02:18:31-08:00</dc:date>
  778.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  779.         <prism:coverDate/>
  780.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  781.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12538</guid>
  782.         <title>Lacking family ties during COVID‐19: A longitudinal, small‐scale qualitative analysis of fictive kin in older adulthood</title>
  783.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  784.         <dc:description>
  785. Abstract
  786. Family support was an important coping mechanism for older adults during the COVID‐19 pandemic. However, not all older adults had access to traditional family ties amid the pandemic. Thus, this study sought to investigate if and how fictive kin ties were developed as a result of pandemic‐related isolation. Older adults (n = 8) who were identified as lacking access to traditional family ties were interviewed five times across 2 years of the pandemic, and their transcripts were analyzed to detect shifts in their fictive kin relationships over time. The majority (n = 5) of participants developed fictive kin ties, either before or during the pandemic, while other older adults struggled with isolation (n = 3). This research has implications that may expand the understanding of social connections and help better support older adults during moments of historical crisis.
  787. </dc:description>
  788.         <content:encoded>
  789. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  790. &lt;p&gt;Family support was an important coping mechanism for older adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. However, not all older adults had access to traditional family ties amid the pandemic. Thus, this study sought to investigate if and how fictive kin ties were developed as a result of pandemic-related isolation. Older adults (&lt;i&gt;n&lt;/i&gt; = 8) who were identified as lacking access to traditional family ties were interviewed five times across 2 years of the pandemic, and their transcripts were analyzed to detect shifts in their fictive kin relationships over time. The majority (&lt;i&gt;n&lt;/i&gt; = 5) of participants developed fictive kin ties, either before or during the pandemic, while other older adults struggled with isolation (&lt;i&gt;n&lt;/i&gt; = 3). This research has implications that may expand the understanding of social connections and help better support older adults during moments of historical crisis.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  791.         <dc:creator>
  792. Bryce Van Vleet,
  793. Heather R. Fuller,
  794. Emily E. Kinkade,
  795. Andrea Huseth‐Zosel
  796. </dc:creator>
  797.         <category>QUALITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  798.         <dc:title>Lacking family ties during COVID‐19: A longitudinal, small‐scale qualitative analysis of fictive kin in older adulthood</dc:title>
  799.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12538</dc:identifier>
  800.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  801.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12538</prism:doi>
  802.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12538?af=R</prism:url>
  803.         <prism:section>QUALITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  804.      </item>
  805.      <item>
  806.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12536?af=R</link>
  807.         <pubDate>Wed, 24 Jan 2024 02:39:08 -0800</pubDate>
  808.         <dc:date>2024-01-24T02:39:08-08:00</dc:date>
  809.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  810.         <prism:coverDate/>
  811.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  812.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12536</guid>
  813.         <title>Transformative power of friendships: Examining the relationships among friendship quality, self‐change, and well‐being</title>
  814.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  815.         <dc:description>
  816. Abstract
  817. Friendships are beneficial to a person's growth and well‐being. People in close relationships may experience four types of self‐change: self‐expansion, self‐pruning, self‐contraction, and self‐adulteration. The current cross‐sectional research sought to investigate whether these relational self‐change processes explain the links between friendship quality and well‐being in same‐sex best friendships. Study 1A (N = 187) adapted the Turkish Relational Self‐Change Scale to the friendship context and confirmed its psychometric adequacy in addressing friendship‐based self‐changes in Türkiye. Study 1B (N = 306) examined the links between different friendship functions and relational self‐changes, as well as whether relational self‐changes explain the link between friendship quality and ontological well‐being. Among the specific friendship functions, stimulating companionship and self‐validation were related to self‐expansion and self‐pruning, whereas reliable alliance was related to self‐contraction and self‐adulteration. Self‐adulteration also indirectly explained the relationship between friendship quality and ontological well‐being. Study 2 (N = 204) revealed that only self‐pruning explained the link between perceived friend responsiveness and overall well‐being. The contributions of friendships to self‐change and personal well‐being were discussed in light of the findings.
  818. </dc:description>
  819.         <content:encoded>
  820. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  821. &lt;p&gt;Friendships are beneficial to a person's growth and well-being. People in close relationships may experience four types of self-change: self-expansion, self-pruning, self-contraction, and self-adulteration. The current cross-sectional research sought to investigate whether these relational self-change processes explain the links between friendship quality and well-being in same-sex best friendships. Study 1A (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 187) adapted the Turkish Relational Self-Change Scale to the friendship context and confirmed its psychometric adequacy in addressing friendship-based self-changes in Türkiye. Study 1B (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 306) examined the links between different friendship functions and relational self-changes, as well as whether relational self-changes explain the link between friendship quality and ontological well-being. Among the specific friendship functions, stimulating companionship and self-validation were related to self-expansion and self-pruning, whereas reliable alliance was related to self-contraction and self-adulteration. Self-adulteration also indirectly explained the relationship between friendship quality and ontological well-being. Study 2 (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 204) revealed that only self-pruning explained the link between perceived friend responsiveness and overall well-being. The contributions of friendships to self-change and personal well-being were discussed in light of the findings.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  822.         <dc:creator>
  823. Emine Yücel,
  824. Duygu Dincer
  825. </dc:creator>
  826.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  827.         <dc:title>Transformative power of friendships: Examining the relationships among friendship quality, self‐change, and well‐being</dc:title>
  828.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12536</dc:identifier>
  829.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  830.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12536</prism:doi>
  831.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12536?af=R</prism:url>
  832.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  833.      </item>
  834.      <item>
  835.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12537?af=R</link>
  836.         <pubDate>Mon, 22 Jan 2024 20:53:34 -0800</pubDate>
  837.         <dc:date>2024-01-22T08:53:34-08:00</dc:date>
  838.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  839.         <prism:coverDate/>
  840.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  841.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12537</guid>
  842.         <title>Attachment avoidance predicts limited and selective sharing of personal events in close relationships</title>
  843.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  844.         <dc:description>
  845. Abstract
  846. Communicating personal experiences is crucial for fostering close relationships, but people can be reluctant to self‐disclose. The current research assessed the extent to which relationship‐specific (RS) and global attachment avoidance limit self‐disclosure of personal events in close relationships. Critically, we also identified the types of personal events that people will selectively share in relationships characterized by higher attachment avoidance. Participants (N = 609 in the aggregated sample) reported whether they shared up to four positive and four negative events that happened to them recently with up to eight close network‐members. We found that a one scale‐point increase in RS or global attachment avoidance (7‐point scales) predicted a 68% decrease or a 28% decrease in the odds of sharing, respectively. Nevertheless, people in more avoidant relationships did share personal events selectively: they uniquely prioritized sharing positive events, events that conveyed personal competence, and events that avoided vulnerability. Our findings shed light on the underlying factors contributing to the low likelihood of sharing personal experiences in more avoidant relationships and offer insights for future intervention work.
  847. </dc:description>
  848.         <content:encoded>
  849. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  850. &lt;p&gt;Communicating personal experiences is crucial for fostering close relationships, but people can be reluctant to self-disclose. The current research assessed the extent to which relationship-specific (RS) and global attachment avoidance limit self-disclosure of personal events in close relationships. Critically, we also identified the types of personal events that people will selectively share in relationships characterized by higher attachment avoidance. Participants (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 609 in the aggregated sample) reported whether they shared up to four positive and four negative events that happened to them recently with up to eight close network-members. We found that a one scale-point increase in RS or global attachment avoidance (7-point scales) predicted a 68% decrease or a 28% decrease in the odds of sharing, respectively. Nevertheless, people in more avoidant relationships did share personal events selectively: they uniquely prioritized sharing positive events, events that conveyed personal competence, and events that avoided vulnerability. Our findings shed light on the underlying factors contributing to the low likelihood of sharing personal experiences in more avoidant relationships and offer insights for future intervention work.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  851.         <dc:creator>
  852. Elina R. Sun,
  853. Brett K. Jakubiak
  854. </dc:creator>
  855.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  856.         <dc:title>Attachment avoidance predicts limited and selective sharing of personal events in close relationships</dc:title>
  857.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12537</dc:identifier>
  858.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  859.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12537</prism:doi>
  860.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12537?af=R</prism:url>
  861.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  862.      </item>
  863.      <item>
  864.         <link>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12535?af=R</link>
  865.         <pubDate>Sun, 14 Jan 2024 21:17:50 -0800</pubDate>
  866.         <dc:date>2024-01-14T09:17:50-08:00</dc:date>
  867.         <source url="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14756811?af=R">Wiley: Personal Relationships: Table of Contents</source>
  868.         <prism:coverDate/>
  869.         <prism:coverDisplayDate/>
  870.         <guid isPermaLink="false">10.1111/pere.12535</guid>
  871.         <title>Is caring for oneself relevant to happy relationship functioning? Exploring associations between self‐compassion and romantic relationship satisfaction in actors and partners</title>
  872.         <description>Personal Relationships, EarlyView. </description>
  873.         <dc:description>
  874. Abstract
  875. Self‐compassion means being supportive and kind to oneself when experiencing failure or inadequacies. It is associated with adaptive intrapersonal and relational outcomes for individuals. This evidence was extended by using an Actor‐Partner Interdependence framework. Other‐sex couples (N = 209) completed measures of self‐compassion, relationship‐specific self‐compassion, and relationship satisfaction. Both self‐compassion measures were related to global relationship satisfaction and facets thereof (e.g., sexuality, engagement, trust) for actors. Relationship‐specific self‐compassion was also positively related to the partner's relationship satisfaction (particularly for men). It is suggested that researchers (a) consider the interdependence of the partners when analyzing self‐compassion in relationships and test for partner effects and (b) use fine‐grained and domain‐specific measures to develop a more complete understanding of self‐compassion's associations with criterion variables.
  876. </dc:description>
  877.         <content:encoded>
  878. &lt;h2&gt;Abstract&lt;/h2&gt;
  879. &lt;p&gt;Self-compassion means being supportive and kind to oneself when experiencing failure or inadequacies. It is associated with adaptive intrapersonal and relational outcomes for individuals. This evidence was extended by using an Actor-Partner Interdependence framework. Other-sex couples (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 209) completed measures of self-compassion, relationship-specific self-compassion, and relationship satisfaction. Both self-compassion measures were related to global relationship satisfaction and facets thereof (e.g., sexuality, engagement, trust) for actors. Relationship-specific self-compassion was also positively related to the partner's relationship satisfaction (particularly for men). It is suggested that researchers (a) consider the interdependence of the partners when analyzing self-compassion in relationships and test for partner effects and (b) use fine-grained and domain-specific measures to develop a more complete understanding of self-compassion's associations with criterion variables.&lt;/p&gt;</content:encoded>
  880.         <dc:creator>
  881. Robert Körner,
  882. Nancy Tandler,
  883. Lars‐Eric Petersen,
  884. Astrid Schütz
  885. </dc:creator>
  886.         <category>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</category>
  887.         <dc:title>Is caring for oneself relevant to happy relationship functioning? Exploring associations between self‐compassion and romantic relationship satisfaction in actors and partners</dc:title>
  888.         <dc:identifier>10.1111/pere.12535</dc:identifier>
  889.         <prism:publicationName>Personal Relationships</prism:publicationName>
  890.         <prism:doi>10.1111/pere.12535</prism:doi>
  891.         <prism:url>https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/pere.12535?af=R</prism:url>
  892.         <prism:section>QUANTITATIVE MANUSCRIPT</prism:section>
  893.      </item>
  894.   </channel>
  895. </rss>
  896.  

If you would like to create a banner that links to this page (i.e. this validation result), do the following:

  1. Download the "valid RSS" banner.

  2. Upload the image to your own server. (This step is important. Please do not link directly to the image on this server.)

  3. Add this HTML to your page (change the image src attribute if necessary):

If you would like to create a text link instead, here is the URL you can use:

http://www.feedvalidator.org/check.cgi?url=http%3A//onlinelibrary.wiley.com/rss/journal/10.1111/%28ISSN%291475-6811

Copyright © 2002-9 Sam Ruby, Mark Pilgrim, Joseph Walton, and Phil Ringnalda